In the intricate dance of biology, few forces shape the body—and mind—as quietly yet profoundly as hormones. Now, a new brain imaging study sheds light on how these internal rhythms may not only guide mood or fertility, but also subtly influence how women perceive and process the people around them—especially other women.
The research, published in the journal Physiology & Behavior, reveals that during the mid-luteal phase of the menstrual cycle—when the hormone progesterone surges—women show greater brain activity and sharper attention when completing tasks involving female faces. Not male faces. Not abstract symbols. But distinctly, and uniquely, the faces of other women.
This finding, say researchers, offers fresh insight into how sex hormones shape the brain’s attention systems—not just in terms of raw cognition, but in deeply social and context-specific ways. It’s a subtle shift, but one that suggests the female brain is attuned to more than we’ve previously imagined.
A New Lens on an Ancient System
Modern science has long known that ovarian hormones like estrogen and progesterone influence brain function. These hormones ebb and flow with the menstrual cycle, affecting everything from mood to memory. But while many studies have explored how these changes affect basic cognitive functions, fewer have examined how hormones interact with the complex, emotionally rich domain of social perception.
“Most research looks at cognition in a vacuum,” said Casey Bennett, lead author of the study and a graduate student at the University of Missouri. “But real life doesn’t happen in isolation. We wanted to know: do these hormonal fluctuations change how women pay attention to socially meaningful stimuli—like faces?”
To answer that question, the team recruited 53 naturally cycling women in their early twenties who weren’t using hormonal birth control. Participants were assigned to two groups based on their cycle phase: the late follicular phase, when progesterone levels are low, and the mid-luteal phase, when they are high. Saliva samples confirmed the participants’ hormonal status.
Then came the test—a socially charged twist on a classic psychological task.
The Faces Behind the Data
Inside an fMRI scanner, participants completed what’s known as a face–gender Stroop task. On the screen, they saw images of neutral male and female faces overlaid with gendered words like “man” or “woman.” Sometimes, the face and label matched (congruent); other times, they clashed (incongruent), requiring participants to ignore the label and focus on the face itself.
It sounds simple—but it’s actually a challenge to the brain’s executive control system, especially when the information conflicts. It tests how well the mind can stay focused under pressure, filtering out distractions and prioritizing the most relevant information.
And what the researchers discovered was striking.
Women in the mid-luteal phase—when progesterone levels peak—were significantly more accurate in identifying female faces during congruent trials. Their reaction times were slightly slower, but only for female faces, suggesting that they were allocating more mental effort and attention to those stimuli.
“The longer response times don’t indicate confusion,” Bennett explained. “They suggest increased cognitive engagement—these participants were thinking harder and more deliberately when processing female faces.”
A Brain Primed for Social Signal
What was happening under the surface matched the behavioral data. Brain scans showed that during the mid-luteal phase, women exhibited greater activation in a key control region known as the inferior frontal gyrus—specifically during trials involving female faces. This region is deeply involved in executive function: attention, inhibition, and cognitive control.
Interestingly, the activation of this brain area scaled with progesterone levels. The higher the hormone, the more the region lit up—especially when participants had to resolve conflicting information. Estradiol, another major female hormone, showed no such correlation.
Resting-state scans—images taken when the brain isn’t performing any task—revealed another layer of the story. In mid-luteal participants, the inferior frontal gyrus played a more central role in the brain’s network, showing higher “betweenness centrality,” a measure of how crucial a region is for connecting disparate brain areas. Even at rest, their brains were wired for control.
Mediation analysis—a statistical method that shows causation chains—confirmed the link: higher progesterone led to increased brain activation, which in turn led to longer reaction times on female face trials. A deliberate, hormone-driven loop of social attention.
Why Female Faces?
Why would the female brain, under the influence of progesterone, focus more intently on female faces? One possibility comes from evolutionary psychology. Some theories propose that progesterone, which rises during the second half of the menstrual cycle to support potential pregnancy, may increase motivations related to social bonding—especially with other women, who might provide support during vulnerable times.
“In many species, and certainly in humans, social support is critical during pregnancy and child-rearing,” said Sarah Jacquet, a co-author and assistant professor of paleontology at the University of Missouri. “It’s possible that heightened progesterone prepares the brain to seek and interpret cues from potential allies—other women.”
Though the current study didn’t test for cooperative behavior directly, the findings suggest the brain is selectively tuning its attentional spotlight, preparing the cognitive landscape for what might come next.
Not Just Any Change—A Socially Specific One
Perhaps most intriguing is the specificity of the effect. The difference wasn’t general attention or general face processing—it was focused on female faces. Male faces didn’t elicit the same brain response or behavioral accuracy, suggesting that the hormone’s influence isn’t global but strategic.
To delve deeper, the researchers applied multivariate pattern analysis, a machine learning approach that examines how well brain regions can distinguish between different task conditions. They found that during the mid-luteal phase, the inferior frontal gyrus more accurately differentiated between congruent and incongruent trials involving female faces. The brain was not only working harder—it was working smarter.
Science with a Caveat
Despite the study’s exciting implications, the authors note several limitations. Each woman was tested only once, meaning individual hormone fluctuations across cycles weren’t tracked in detail. A within-subject design—testing the same participants across multiple phases—could offer more robust conclusions.
Hormone levels were confirmed by saliva samples, but ovulation was estimated through calendars and self-report. Future studies could improve precision using ovulation kits or continuous temperature tracking. Finally, while the researchers interpreted the findings in the context of social bonding, the task didn’t directly assess affiliative or cooperative behavior.
Still, the results open a powerful new window into how biology, cognition, and social motivation intertwine.
The Takeaway: A Smarter, More Social Brain
This study doesn’t suggest that women are “better” or “worse” at attention depending on their cycle. Instead, it paints a richer, more nuanced picture of how the female brain is constantly adapting—not just to the environment, but to the inner hormonal symphony playing within.
Hormones like progesterone don’t just affect the reproductive system—they ripple through attention networks, shape social sensitivity, and perhaps even alter the mind’s readiness to connect with others. What was once invisible is now being mapped, image by image, scan by scan.
As neuroscience continues to unravel the mysteries of the brain, one thing becomes increasingly clear: it’s not just what we think or feel—but when we think or feel it—that shapes how we see the world.
And sometimes, the rhythm of that world starts from within.
Love this? Share it and help us spark curiosity about science!